256 research outputs found

    A linguistic analysis of lying in negative evaluations: The speech act performance of Chinese learners of Korean

    Get PDF
    ์ด ๋…ผ๋ฌธ์€ ์ค‘๊ตญ์ธ ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ํ•™์Šต์ž์™€ ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ํ™”์ž๋“ค ์‚ฌ์ด์˜ โ€˜๊ฑฐ์ง“๋งโ€™ ํ™”ํ–‰ ์–‘์ƒ์„ ์–ธ์–ดํ•™์ ์œผ๋กœ ๋ถ„์„ํ•œ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ์ด๋‹ค. ์—ฌ๊ธฐ์„œ ๋งํ•˜๋Š” โ€˜๊ฑฐ์ง“๋งโ€™์ด๋ž€ ์š”์ฒญ, ์‚ฌ๊ณผ, ๊ฑฐ์ ˆ ๋“ฑ๊ณผ ๊ฐ™์€ ํ™”ํ–‰์˜ ์ผ์ข…์œผ๋กœ์„œ โ€˜๋ถ€์ •์  ํ‰๊ฐ€โ€™์— ์†ํ•˜๋ฉฐ ๋Œ€ํ™” ์ฐธ์—ฌ์ž๋‚˜ ์ƒํ™ฉ์„ ๊ณ ๋ คํ•œ ์†Œ์œ„ โ€˜์„ ์˜์˜ ๊ฑฐ์ง“๋งโ€™์„ ๊ฐ€๋ฆฌํ‚ค๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์œผ๋กœ ์ดํ•ดํ•  ์ˆ˜ ์žˆ์„ ๊ฒƒ์ด๋‹ค. ์šฐ๋ฆฌ๋Š” ์ค‘๊ตญ์ธ ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ํ•™์Šต์ž 15๋ช…๊ณผ ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ํ™”์ž 15๋ช…์„ ๋Œ€์ƒ์œผ๋กœ ๋‹ดํ™”์™„์„ฑํ…Œ์ŠคํŠธ(DCT)์™€ ๋ถ€์—ฐ์„ค๋ช…์งˆ๋ฌธ์ง€(QFE)๋ฅผ ์‚ฌ์šฉํ•˜์—ฌ ํ”ผ์‹คํ—˜์ž๋“ค์˜ ํ™”ํ–‰์„ ๋ถ„์„ํ•˜์˜€๋‹ค. ํ”ผ์‹คํ—˜์ž ์ž์‹ ๋“ค์˜ ์„ค๋ช…๊ณผ ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด๊ต์œก ์ „๋ฌธ๊ฐ€ ๋‹ค์„ฏ ๋ช…์˜ ํŒ์ •์„ ์ข…ํ•ฉํ•ด โ€˜๊ฑฐ์ง“๋งโ€™ ํ™”ํ–‰์„ ๊ฐ€๋ ค๋‚ด๊ณ  ํ†ต๊ณ„ ์ฒ˜๋ฆฌ๋ฅผ ๋ฐ”ํƒ•์œผ๋กœ ๋‹ค์Œ๊ณผ ๊ฐ™์€ ๊ฒฐ๋ก ์— ๋„๋‹ฌํ–ˆ๋‹ค. ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ํ™”์ž๋“ค์ด ์ค‘๊ตญ์ธ ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ํ•™์Šต์ž๋“ค๋ณด๋‹ค (์„ ์˜์˜) ๊ฑฐ์ง“๋ง์„ ๋” ๋งŽ์ด ์ˆ˜ํ–‰ํ•˜๋Š” ๊ฒƒ์œผ๋กœ ๋‚˜ํƒ€๋‚ฌ๋‹ค. ๊ทธ๋ฆฌ๊ณ  ๋‘ ์ง‘๋‹จ ๋ชจ๋‘ ๋ถ€์ •์  ํ‰๊ฐ€๊ฐ€ ์‚ฌ๋ฌผ์— ๊ด€๋ จ๋œ ๊ฒฝ์šฐ๋ณด๋‹ค ์‚ฌ๋žŒ์— ๊ด€๋ จ๋œ ๊ฒฝ์šฐ์— โ€˜๊ฑฐ์ง“๋งโ€™ ํ™”ํ–‰์„ ๋” ๋งŽ์ด ์‚ฌ์šฉํ•œ๋‹ค. ๊ทธ๋Ÿฌ๋‚˜ ํ™”์ž์™€ ์ฒญ์ž ์‚ฌ์ด์˜ ์นœ์†Œ๊ด€๊ณ„(distance)๋‚˜ ์ƒํ•˜๊ด€๊ณ„(power)๋Š” ๊ฑฐ์ง“๋ง ์‚ฌ์šฉ์— ์ง์ ‘์  ์ƒ๊ด€ ๊ด€๊ณ„๋ฅผ ๋ณด์—ฌ์ฃผ์ง€ ์•Š์•˜๋‹ค. ์ด ์—ฐ๊ตฌ๋Š” ์ง€๊ธˆ๊นŒ์ง€ ํ™”ํ–‰ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ ์ค‘์—์„œ ์ƒ๋Œ€์ ์œผ๋กœ ์—ฐ๊ตฌ๊ฐ€ ๋ถ€์ง„ํ–ˆ๋˜ ๋ถ€์ •ํ‰๊ฐ€์™€ โ€˜๊ฑฐ์ง“๋งโ€™ ํ™”ํ–‰์— ๋Œ€ํ•œ ๋ถ„์„์„ ์‹œ๋„ํ–ˆ๋‹ค๋Š” ์ ์—์„œ ์˜๋ฏธ๊ฐ€ ์žˆ๋‹ค. ๋˜ํ•œ ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ํ™”์ž์™€ ์ค‘๊ตญ์ธ ํ•œ๊ตญ์–ด ํ•™์Šต์ž ์‚ฌ์ด์— ๋ณด์ด๋Š” ํ™”ํ–‰ ์ˆ˜ํ–‰์˜ ์ฐจ์ด๋ฅผ ๋ฌธํ™”์ธ์‹(cultural awareness)์˜ ๊ด€์ ์—์„œ ํ•ด์„ํ•ด ๋ณผ ์ˆ˜ ์žˆ๋Š” ๊ฐ€๋Šฅ์„ฑ๋„ ์—ด์–ด ์ฃผ์—ˆ๋‹ค

    Introduction

    Get PDF
    Why do parent\u2013child argumentative interactions matter? What is the reason for such an interest? This chapter provides the reasons that motivated the study of parent\u2013child argumentation with the aim to understand the function of this type of interactions. Focusing on the activity of family mealtime, in the first part, the chapter draws attention to the distinctive features of parent\u2013child conversations. A second section of the chapter is devoted to discussing whether and, eventually, when children have the competence to construct arguments and engage in argumentative discussions with the aim to convince their parents to change opinion. In the last part of the chapter, research questions and structure of the volume are presented

    Requests On E-Mail: a Cross-Cultural Comparison

    Full text link
    This study investigates differences in request e-mails written in English by Chinese English learners and native American English speakers The results show that while Chinese English learners treat e-mail communications like either formal letters or telephone conversations, native American English speakers regard e-mail communications as closer to written memos It was also found that although the native American English speakers structure their e-mail request messages in a rather direct sequence, the linguistic forms they employ to express their requests are more indirect In contrast, the Chinese English learners structure their request messages in an indirect sequence, but the linguistic forms they use to realize their requests are more direct Given this contrast, it is not surprising that some of the request samples written by Chinese English learners were judged as very impolite by the native English speaking evaluators in this study The findings of this study thus demonstrate the importance of studying requests within the overall discourse in which they occur. Studying only the linguistic forms used in phrasing the request itself, as in the studies conducted by Blum-Kulka et al (1989), cannot provide us with a full picture of the cultural differences inherent in making requestsPeer Reviewedhttp://deepblue.lib.umich.edu/bitstream/2027.42/69055/2/10.1177_003368829802900206.pd

    The Initial Phase of the Argumentative Discussions Between Parents and Children

    Get PDF
    This chapter examines the initial phase of parent\u2013child argumentative discussions during mealtime. The conceptual tool adopted for the analysis is based on the pragma-dialectical ideal model of a critical discussion (van Eemeren & Grootendorst\u201a 2004). The types of issues leading parents and children to engage in argumentative discussions during mealtime as well as the contribution that parents and children provide to the inception of argumentation are described and discussed. The analysis of the initial phase of parent\u2013child argumentative discussions also considers the role played by the specificity of the parent\u2013child relationship and the distinctive features of the activity of family mealtime for the beginning of an argumentative discussion. Exemplary argumentative sequences that bring to light the results obtained through the qualitative analysis of a larger corpus of argumentative discussions between parents and children are presented and discussed

    A Qualitative Methodology for Studying Parentโ€“Child Argumentation

    Get PDF
    This chapter provides a detailed exposรฉ of the research methodology on which the investigation of parentโ€“child argumentation during mealtime is based. In the first part, the conceptual tools adopted for the analysis of argumentative discussions between parents and children, i.e., the pragma-dialectical ideal model of a critical discussion and the Argumentum Model of Topics, are presented. Subsequently, the process of data gathering and the procedures for the transcription of oral data are discussed. Finally, in the last part of the chapter, ethical issues and practical problems in collecting parentโ€“child mealtime conversations present throughout the study are considered

    Reporting conditionals with modals

    Get PDF
    Conditionals and modals work in tandem in some instances of practical reasoning, or decision making. Consider the following example (from Kratzer 2012): a. I want to become a mayor. b. (q) I will become a mayor only if (p) I go to the pub. c. Therefore, I should go to the pub. Given what the cogniser wants (a) and the relevant circumstances (b), the conclusion that the cogniser goes to the pub comes out as necessary. Hence, the presence of the necessity modal should in (c). Indeed, given the context of (a), the necessity modal in (c) is simply a reflection of the necessity of p for q, which is overtly represented by the use of the โ€˜only if p, qโ€™ construction. This chapter looks into whether indirect reports of conditionals โ€“ in particular, indirect reports which involve the use of a modal verb โ€“ are sensitive to the necessity of p for q in cases where necessity is not overtly represented in a conditional, as in โ€˜if p, qโ€™ formulations. We report on two online experiments into the relation between (i) perceived necessity or sufficiency of the truth of a conditional antecedent for the truth of the consequent, and (ii) the formulation of an indirect report of a conditional with necessity or possibility modals (have to, should, could). In Experiment 1, the โ€˜necessity/sufficiency of p for qโ€™ variable was manipulated by contextually altering the number of alternative antecedents (e.g. Cummins et al. 1991; Thompson 1994; Politzer 2003). It was found that modals used in indirect reports of โ€˜if p, qโ€™ conditionals co-vary with the number of alternative antecedents in predictable ways. This suggests that modals used in indirect reports of โ€˜if p, qโ€™ conditionals may be a diagnostic for biconditional versus material interpretations of conditionals. The aim of Experiment 2 was to find out whether the results of Experiment 1 could be replicated in contexts which lower/eliminate the believability of the conditionals. It was found that manipulating the believability variable has no reliable effect on the results

    Indirect Reports in Modern Eastern Armenian

    Get PDF
    In this work we consider the distribution of complementizers in Modern Eastern Armenian. There are two complementizers: wor and tโ€˜e. They both introduce complement clauses, but tโ€˜e also expresses a dubitative value, implying that the speaker has doubts on the content following the complementizer. Moreover, tโ€˜e, when embedded under verbs of saying, shifts the anchoring of indexicals, moving the anchor from the speaker โ€“ better called utterer โ€“ to the subject of the saying predicate. On the basis of this and further evidence coming from the analysis of sequence of tense and if-clauses, we will argue that the position of tโ€˜e in the left periphery of the clause occupies a high position in the syntactic hierarchy. The aim of this work is on one hand, a better understanding of indirect reports and their syntax and, on the other, a more precise characterization of indexicals across languages

    Shifting Characterizations of the โ€˜Common Peopleโ€™ in Modern English Retranslations of Thucydidesโ€™ History of the Peloponnesian War: A corpus-based analysis

    Get PDF
    Little research has yet explored the impact of (re)translation on narrative characterization, that is, on the process through which the various actors depicted in a narrative are attributed particular traits and qualities. Moreover, the few studies that have been published on this topic are either rather more anecdotal than systematic, or their focus is primarily on the losses in character information that inevitably occur when a narrative is retold for a new audience in a new linguistic context. They do not explore how the translatorโ€™s own background knowledge and ideological beliefs might affect the characterization process for readers of their target-language text. Consequently, this paper seeks to make two contributions to the field: first, it presents a corpus-based methodology developed as part of the Genealogies of Knowledge project for the comparative analysis of characterization patterns in multiple retranslations of a single source text. Such an approach is valuable, it is argued, because it can enhance our ability to engage in a more systematic manner with the accumulation of characterization cues spread throughout a narrative. Second, the paper seeks to move discussions of the effects of translation on narrative characterization away from a paradigm of loss, deficiency and failure, promoting instead a perspective which embraces the productive role translators often play in reconfiguring the countless narratives through which we come to know, imagine and make sense of the past, our present and futures. The potential of this methodology and theoretical standpoint is illustrated through a case study exploring changes in the characterization of โ€˜the common peopleโ€™ in two English-language versions of classical Greek historian Thucydidesโ€™ History of the Peloponnesian War, the first produced by Samuel Bloomfield in 1829 and the second by Steven Lattimore in 1998. Particular attention is paid to the referring expressions used by each translatorโ€”such as the multitude vs. the common peopleโ€”as well as the specific attributes assigned to this narrative actor. In this way, the study attempts to gain deeper insight into the ways in which these translations reflect important shifts in attitudes within key political debates concerning the benefits and dangers of democracy
    • โ€ฆ
    corecore